We explored this topic—in part—because a Bitch reader asked us to look into it. Got a question about feminism and pop culture that you want answered, too? Tell us!
I have a good friend with whom I often talk about politics and culture. He’s a lovely, kind person with a deep commitment to social justice and an unfortunate tendency to mansplain. He knows this, and is trying to work on it, so we developed a sort of code. Whenever he starts mansplaining, I just say, “Ow! My ladybrain!” It’s good-natured ribbing that gets the point across, but it also winkingly refers to ingrained cultural notions of the differences between male and female brains, notions that have been used to justify the subjugation of women.
Whether the brains of men and women actually vary in form and function is a hotly debated question in psychology and neuroscience.
Cultural discussion of gender and brains tends toward a reductive,
binary view of neurobiology: Men are more aggressive and sex-focused,
and their brains tend toward building systems; women are more emotional
and communicative, and they tend toward empathy. Pop culture is rife
with this
men/women-are-just-soooooo-alien-how-can-I-possibly-understand-them-without-help
idea. It is the basis for many stale stand-up routines and films, of course, and for a litany of books about relationships. John Gray’s 1992 Men Are from Mars, Women Are from Venus
is perhaps the best-known example and is now fully embedded in the
cultural lexicon (thoroughly unpacking the problems with this book and
its legacy is a heroic endeavor that I don’t have room for here).
If
your feminist alarm bells are ringing, that’s good. The science of
whether sex determines anything about brain structure and function is
actually far more complicated than the simplistic view put forth by,
say, Louann Brizendine, author of the books The Female Brain (2006) and The Male Brain (2010), both of which were packed with messy, often unsupported assertions and inferences. As Harvard molecular and cellular biology professor Catherine Dulac explained to Scientific American last year,
“[I]t is assumed that the male and the female brains are very different
because male and female behaviors differ so significantly. But over the
last few decades, neuroscientists have been looking for major
anatomical differences and did not find that many.”
A buzzed-about recent study, titled “Sex Beyond the Genitalia: The Human Brain Mosaic,” helps debunk the notion that there are distinct male and female brains. The 2015 study
shows that brains cannot be categorized as male or female based on
their structure. Instead, we should see brains as a big, heterogeneous
group, and each brain as a “mosaic” that may have any combination of
characteristics. Our brains are one more place where gender is best seen
as a spectrum—not a cut-and-dry binary. “[M]ost brains are comprised of
unique ‘mosaics’ of features, some more common in females compared with
males, some more common in males compared with females, and some common
in both females and males,” notes the study’s designer Daphna Joel, a
professor in the school of Psychological Sciences at Tel-Aviv
University.
Undermining
the commonly held belief that males and females have different brains
is important because sex differences between brains has been used as a
justification for social differences and inequalities. There’s a long
tradition of biological research being used to justify racism, sexism, ethnocentrism,
and a very long list of other forms of oppressive social organization.
When biology and social differences are linked, those social differences
are often thought to be biologically determined. For instance, the
18th-century practice of phrenology used measurements and classification
of human skulls to “prove” the superiority of Caucasians. Later, the
fact that women tend to have smaller brains than men would be used as a reason to prevent them from attending college and arguing (as late as 2006!) that women are just not as smart as men because we have smaller brains.
Biological determinism is a slippery slope that starts with
observations of differences between people and slides into a belief that
the ways society is structured around those differences are normal,
natural, and inevitable. It also tends to lead to a fatalistic view:
This is just the way things are.
The
mosaic-brain theory pushes against these ideas. It says that there are
differences between brain structures, sure, but it would be more
accurate and useful to think of all brains as part of one group that
displays a wide variety of anatomical characteristics. The conclusion
often drawn from previous neuroscientific research was that differences between
groups of male and female brains were greater than differences within
those groups. The mosaic brain study argues that all human brains exist
on a spectrum and even suggests that sex should not be used as a variable in scientific studies
of the brain because “comparing brains of females to brains of males
would be analogous to comparing two samples randomly drawn from a single
population of brains.”
Of
course, not everyone agrees with this conclusion. Other researchers and
folks in the field published responses to the study that challenged its
methods, premises, and conclusions. One common critique was that the study focused on brain anatomy rather than brain function
and therefore could not detect sex-based differences in how brains
actually work. Another critic was troubled by the idea that in order for
a brain to be classified as “male” or “female” according to Joel et
al.’s definition, it would have to be entirely so and could not display
any characteristics of the other classification, which the critic thought unreasonable.
As a response to these critiques, Joel published a follow-up opinion piece
coauthored with Anne Fausto-Sterling, a biologist and professor in the
Department of Molecular Biology, Cell Biology, and Biochemistry at Brown
University who is known for her work challenging binary understandings
of biological sex. Joel and Fausto-Sterling clarify the goals and
outcomes of the research: They are not denying that differences between
brains exist, they are just challenging the accuracy and utility of
funneling those differences into two sex-based categories.
This study, unsurprisingly, in a culture that is deeply invested in binaristic views of sex and gender, is not without its detractors, and some of them seem to raise valid critiques of the science. Ultimately, though, the study seems like a step in a good direction. Research that flies in the face of biological determinism and moves toward dismantling the sex and gender binaries can only be a good thing, even when it’s flawed.
This study, unsurprisingly, in a culture that is deeply invested in binaristic views of sex and gender, is not without its detractors, and some of them seem to raise valid critiques of the science. Ultimately, though, the study seems like a step in a good direction. Research that flies in the face of biological determinism and moves toward dismantling the sex and gender binaries can only be a good thing, even when it’s flawed.
No comments:
Post a Comment